Loading Video...

Child DevelopmentChild Development

Jean Piaget's Impact on Cognitive Development and Education

This episode uncovers Jean Piaget's journey from biologist to psychologist and how his theory transformed developmental psychology. We discuss the stages of cognitive development, his concepts of schemas and assimilation, and his lasting influence on education, from classrooms to homeschooling. Critiques and modern applications reveal why Piaget’s work remains central to how we understand and nurture learning today.

Published OnMarch 18, 2025
Chapter 1

Piaget’s Cognitive Development Theory

Tracey

So, let’s jump right into it. Jean Piaget—one of the most influential minds in developmental psychology, right? Funny enough, he didn’t even start out as a psychologist.

Eric

Exactly. He was actually a biologist at first, studying mollusks, of all things. That attention to meticulous patterns and development? It really laid the groundwork for his later work in child psychology.

Tracey

Oh, that’s fascinating! And somehow, from studying snails, he moved on to studying kids. But, honestly, that shift makes sense. You can see how he turned his eye toward understanding how humans evolve in their thinking over time.

Eric

Absolutely. And evolve they do, through four distinct stages, as he proposed. Each one represents a qualitative leap in how kids understand the world around them.

Tracey

Right, like that first stage—the Sensorimotor stage—babies are basically little scientists, figuring things out through trial and error. And object permanence, oh my goodness, that’s such a huge milestone.

Eric

It really is. For those listening, that’s when a baby learns that objects continue to exist, even when they’re out of sight. It’s why a game of peekaboo works like magic to them at first but loses its charm after a while.

Tracey

Oh, totally! That reminds me—the other day, my nephew was playing hide-and-seek, and guess what? He found me hiding behind the curtain. I was genuinely amazed at how confidently he crawled to me. I’m over here thinking, “Wow, Piaget would applaud this moment.”

Eric

Exactly. And those seemingly small moments show the transition infants make during this stage. Now, after Sensorimotor, things start getting symbolic in the Preoperational stage. Picture a child playing pretend, using a spoon as a sword—

Tracey

Or a block as, like, a phone for their imaginary conversations!

Eric

Exactly. That's symbolic thinking, where they use one object to represent another. But their reasoning is still pretty limited—there's a lot of trial-and-error in how they understand the world. And, of course, egocentrism is huge here; they mostly see things from their own perspective.

Tracey

And then we move into the Concrete Operational stage. This is where things really click for problem-solving in tangible ways, right? Kids finally get stuff like conservation—

Eric

Yes! Conservation. For example, they start recognizing that pouring some juice from a short, wide glass into a tall, thin one doesn’t create more juice. It’s about focusing on logic, but mainly with things they can physically see and touch.

Tracey

And then the pièce de résistance, the Formal Operational stage—this is where abstract thinking comes into play.

Eric

Exactly. Adolescents and adults can reason hypothetically, think about complex ideas like justice or morality, and solve equations without needing objects right in front of them. That ability to think abstractly really separates this stage from the earlier ones.

Tracey

It all sounds so structured, doesn’t it? But I love that Piaget didn’t see these stages as rigid milestones. He knew not every child followed the exact same timeline.

Eric

Absolutely. Children may exhibit characteristics of multiple stages simultaneously, depending on their personal experiences and environments. That ability to adapt—assimilating and then sometimes overhauling how they think as they encounter new challenges—is what keeps development moving forward.

Chapter 2

Key Concepts in Piaget’s Theory

Tracey

Alright, so if we think about those stages of development we just talked about, here’s where schemas really come into play. They’re like mental frameworks we use to make sense of the world at any stage—kinda like a mental filing cabinet where you encounter something new and slot it into an existing file.

Eric

Exactly. Say a child knows what a dog is—they’ve got a schema for “pets.” When they meet a neighbor’s cat, they might think, “Oh, this must be another dog!” That’s assimilation. They’re taking the new experience and fitting it into what they already understand.

Tracey

But then accommodation kicks in, right? They realize, hold on, this ‘dog’ isn’t barking—oh, and it’s climbing the curtains! Suddenly, they’re creating a new schema for cats altogether.

Eric

Exactly. And this process—switching between assimilation and accommodation—is what Piaget called equilibration. It’s like a balancing act. When something doesn’t fit, kids have to rework their understanding to make sense of it all again.

Tracey

Oh, that’s so true! It’s like the brain’s version of trial and error, isn’t it?

Eric

Precisely! Piaget saw children as active learners. They’re not just sponges soaking up information—they’re more like little scientists, constantly experimenting and adapting.

Tracey

Absolutely. And his focus on active learning—on kids exploring and figuring stuff out for themselves—is so foundational in education today. I mean, modern classrooms are full of hands-on, constructivist learning approaches because of this idea.

Eric

Right. And there’s such a strong emphasis on concrete experiences in the early stages. Take his conservation tasks, for instance. If you fill two glasses with the same amount of water and pour one into a tall, skinny glass, younger kids might think the tall one has more water.

Tracey

Oh, yeah. Because they’re focused on the height of the glass, not the quantity.

Eric

Exactly. But as they enter the Concrete Operational stage, they’ll start to understand that quantity doesn’t change just because the shape does. Activities like these really show how their cognitive abilities evolve with hands-on demonstrations.

Tracey

It reminds me of watching kids learn through play—how they gradually start questioning their assumptions and problem-solving. It’s honestly amazing to see that growth happen in real-time.

Chapter 3

Critiques and Modern Applications

Tracey

Building off all of that, it’s clear Piaget’s theory has been groundbreaking in understanding child development. But, as with any major theory, it hasn’t escaped criticism. Some argue he might’ve underestimated what kids are really capable of.

Eric

Absolutely. One of the biggest criticisms is that he underestimated infants’ cognitive abilities. Modern research has shown that even very young babies have a much stronger grasp of the world around them than Piaget originally thought.

Tracey

Really? Like, what kind of stuff have we discovered?

Eric

Well, for instance, object permanence—Piaget pegged it as emerging around eight months, but newer studies suggest infants start to get it much earlier. It’s fascinating when you think about how far cognitive research has come since Piaget’s time.

Tracey

Oh, totally. And isn’t there also this critique about how his theory doesn’t account for cultural differences? Like, how kids’ environments might tweak their cognitive growth?

Eric

That’s right. His studies were largely based on children from Western, middle-class backgrounds, so his findings don’t always apply universally. Cultural factors can play huge roles in shaping how kids develop. Take storytelling, for instance—some cultures use it as a primary teaching tool, which could influence how abstract thinking develops.

Tracey

Ah, that makes sense. And what about the emotional side? I read somewhere that his work didn’t really dig into how emotions impact learning.

Eric

Exactly. That’s another area where Piaget’s work falls a bit short. Emotional and social development weren’t his focus, which leaves a gap if you’re looking at the whole child. The interplay between cognition and emotions is huge when it comes to real-world learning.

Tracey

But, even with these gaps, it’s wild how much of his work is still shaping classrooms today. Like, hands-on learning and all those inquiry-based approaches? Straight out of the Piaget playbook.

Eric

Oh, for sure. I was just at an education conference last month, and Piaget’s name came up constantly. One group was discussing how homeschooling parents are adapting his ideas, using real-world experiences to teach concepts. The flexibility of applying his framework is kind of timeless.

Tracey

Exactly! I feel like any time you see kids piecing knowledge together—whether it’s in a classroom or at home—you’re seeing his influence. And honestly, it just speaks to how forward-thinking he was.

Eric

That’s the thing, isn’t it? His framework may not be perfect, but it’s a starting point that’s inspired heaps of research and innovation. And those criticisms we just talked about? They’ve only deepened our understanding of development.

Tracey

So, in a way, all the debates about his theory just show how relevant it still is. I mean, people don’t critique work that isn’t worth discussing, right?

Eric

Exactly. And I think that’s the beauty of Piaget’s legacy—it pushes us to keep questioning, keep exploring. Learning’s never static, no matter how old you are.

Tracey

On that note, we hope you’ve enjoyed this deep dive into Piaget’s world! It’s been so much fun unpacking his ideas and seeing how they still resonate today.

Eric

Absolutely. And thanks for joining us—I always look forward to these chats. Until next time, keep exploring, and we’ll catch you soon.

About the podcast

A Podcast all about different child Development theories in early years education

This podcast is brought to you by Jellypod, Inc.

© 2025 All rights reserved.